A high school math class experience
is likely one of the more polarizing topics of conversation. You can mention
your experiences in a math class and be guaranteed that someone will explain
how they hated math in school and now avoid it at all costs. However, research is
shedding some light on just how important math is to our daily lives. Low math
achievement has a profoundly negative association in an individual’s life
outcomes. Individuals are more likely locked-out of advanced math courses if they were unsuccessful in earlier classes. Job opportunities are less likely to go to those without proper math credentials. On top of that, poor math skills are
tied to poor financial planning (Bynner and Parsons, 2006).
Unfortunately, math anxiety is associated with low achievement in math classes. Not only is math anxiety associated with
avoidance, but it is also linked to impairment in a child’s ability to think about
math in a way that would help them solve complex problems (Ramirez et al.
2016). It is important to understand why some kids struggle with math because of
the foundational nature of skills learned early on.
Present study:
Working memory is an important ability
to store and manipulate pertinent information while ignoring confusing or
extraneous information. Math Anxiety introduces negative thoughts that make it difficult
to access working memory while solving math problems. Children initially use
simple problem solving strategies, like finger counting, when they start
learning math, but usually move on to more memory-based advanced strategies as
they develop. Examples of these memory-based strategies include decomposition
and retrieval. Decomposition is breaking a problem down into manageable steps,
and retrieval is remembering a particular fact needed for the answer. These
strategies seem easy to adults who’ve had a lot of practice, but for kids these
strategies place a lot of demand on working memory. Decomposition and retrieval,
require long term memory recall and keeping tracking a series of different
steps while ignoring inapplicable answers that are competing for their
attention.
Ramirez et al. looked at the levels
of math anxiety, the use of problem solving strategies, and working memory
capacity in 564 first and second grade children. The researchers wanted to
understand more about how math anxiety effects a child’s problem solving
strategies when working on math problems. Ramirez et al. hypothesized that children with
math anxiety are less likely to use memory-based strategies. To test this
theory, children were given a math anxiety questionnaire, a working memory
task, a math assessment, and a task that assessed what type of strategy they
used to solve a word problem without pencil or paper.
In theory, higher
working memory capacity in children is associated with more advanced memory-based strategy use,
like those listed above, because they have a larger pool of memory resources to use. However,
Ramirez et al. found that children who reported high math anxiety and scored
high on working memory were less likely to use advanced memory-based strategies.
The assumption was that math anxious children with high working memory try to
use the memory-based strategies at first, but their math anxiety interrupts
that process. As a result, high math anxious children are less likely to
develop math skills because they remain reliant on simpler problem solving
strategies. Ramirez et al. expressed concern that children that who scored high
on working memory are more likely to be disproportionately impaired by the
effects of math anxiety, since they might otherwise be capable to solve more
difficult problems.
Going Forward
What can we take from this? Having a
better understanding of how math anxiety is correlated to working memory and
how it influences math achievement is critical in the development of
appropriate interventions. We want the best programs to help our kids who
struggle in school. Ramirez et al. suggest that once math anxiety is
appropriately dealt with, math anxious kids with high working memory are more
likely to use advanced memory-based strategies in the classroom. Being able to
use those advanced strategies goes with higher math achievement in the
classroom. As I mentioned above, there
are negative effects of having poor math skills. The bright side of that
relationship is that gaining more math skills early on is linked to more
positive outcomes in employment, school, and financial stability later on in
life.
Ramirez et al. were also hopeful about
anxiety interventions like cognitive reappraisal that could be modified for children’s
math anxiety. Originally for adults, cognitive reappraisal challenges people to
think about negative stimulus in a way that is less threatening. Ramirez et al. viewed this as a
promising intervention in math anxiety. Whether
through this or other strategies, a strong argument can be made that early
intervention in a math anxious child’s life has the potential to lay a solid
foundation for future achievement.
References:
Bynner,J.,andParsons,S.(2006). Does numeracy matter
more? London: National Research and Development Centre for Adult Literacy and
Numeracy
Ramirez, G., Chang, H., Maloney, E. A., Levine,
S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2016). On the relationship between math anxiety
and math achievement in early elementary school: The role of problem solving
strategies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141,
83–100. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.014
Author: Melissa Elfers
No comments:
Post a Comment